Early Education for Spatial Intelligence: Why, What, and How

نویسنده

  • Nora S. Newcombe
چکیده

Spatial representation and thinking have evolutionary importance for anymobile organism. In addition, they help reasoning in domains that are not obviously spatial, for example, through the use of graphs and diagrams. This article reviews the literature suggesting that mental spatial transformation abilities, while present in some precursory form in infants, toddlers, and preschool children, also undergo considerable development and show important individual differences, which are malleable. These findings provide the basis for thinking about how to promote spatial thinking in preschools, at home, and in children’s play. Integrating spatial content into formal and informal instruction could not only improve spatial functioning in general but also reduce differences related to gender and socioeconomic status that may impede full participation in a technological society. Spatial thinking is often difficult. People frequently get lost or give directions that are difficult to follow or that contain mistakes. They get frustratedwhen attempting to put together ‘‘easy to assemble’’ furniture, and they yell at each other when trying to pack a small car for a long trip. However, such problems may seem like minor hassles when viewed against really important issues, such as illiteracy or failures to graduate high school. The attention of psychologists, educational researchers, and teachers is frequently focused on the basic skills of reading andwriting,mathematics, and science. Should some of this energy also be devoted to improving spatial thinking? There are several crucial reasons why. First, remember that spatial intelligence has evolutionary and adaptive importance. Any mobile organism must be able 1Department of Psychology, Temple University Address correspondence to Nora S. Newcombe, Department of Psychology, Weiss Hall, 1701 North 13th Street, Philadelphia, PA 191226085; e-mail: [email protected]. to navigate in its world to survive and must represent the spatial environment in order to do so. Moving further along the evolutionary timeline, the human ability to make tools is one of the hallmarks of our species. In order to create a successful tool, one must first imagine a shape that is relevant to a particular function, such as cutting or digging, and then fashion that shape out of larger forms. Second, in line with this analysis of evolutionary demands, over a century of research in the psychology of intelligence and cognitive processes has established that spatial thinking is the principal complement to verbal thinking. Several examples drawn from multiple research traditions illustrate this point. Factor analytic research has shown that visualization is a well-defined component skill within general intelligence in adults (Carroll, 1993). Developmental testing shows a spatial/numeracy factor in preschool children (Bornstein, 2009) as well as in chimpanzees (Herrmann, Hernández-Lloreda, Call, Hare, & Tomasello, 2010). Spatial intelligence was one of the types of intelligence proposed in multiple-intelligence theory (Gardner, 1983). Approaches to working memory have distinguished between verbal working memory and the visuospatial sketchpad (Baddeley, 1986). Of course, controversy exists over some of these matters: for example, see McGrew (2009) on the psychometric approach to the structure of intellect, Waterhouse (2006) for a critique of multiple intelligences, and Kane et al. (2004) for evidence that domain specificity may not characterize working memory. Nevertheless, although the architecture of the human mind/brain has yet to be defined definitively, spatial functioningwill likely be a relevant element in the solution. Third, spatial thinking helps reasoning in domains that are not, on the surface, obviously spatial. For example, spatial metaphors and diagrams can be used to understand ordered relations (e.g., the ranking of Gross National Product among developing countries) or complex hierarchical relations (e.g., social relationships and biological taxonomies). Venn diagrams are used to solve logical problems. Maps do more than justshowuswheretogo; theybecometools for thinking intheir  2010 the Authors 102 Journal Compilation 2010 International Mind, Brain, and Education Society and Blackwell Publishing, Inc. Volume 4—Number 3 Nora S. Newcombe and Andrea Frick capacity to display to us the distribution of variables such as population density or natural and economic resources. In fact, one of themost famous stories in epidemiology concerns amap prepared by a London physician, John Snow, during a cholera epidemic. At the time, the way in which cholera spread was unknown. Snow’s map plotted the location of water pumps and the location of cholera cases and revealed how cases clustered around one particular contaminated pump. Fourth, a critically important application of spatial thinking is to the science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) disciplines. For example,Watson andCrick’s discovery of the structure ofDNAoccurredwhen theywere able to fit a three-dimensional model to Rosalind Franklin’s flat images of themolecule—clearly a spatial task. Similarly, a geoscientist visualizes the processes that affect the formation of the earth, an engineer anticipates how various forces may affect the design of a structure, and a neurosurgeon visualizes particular brain areas from magnetic resonance imaging that may determine the outcome of a surgical procedure. Progress and performance in various STEM fields thus seem to be strongly tied to improving people’s ability to reason about spatial configurations and theirproperties. There is real evidence tobackup this proposition. Children and adolescentswho have higher spatial skills in middle and high school are more likely to major in the STEMdisciplines in college and topursueSTEMcareers (Shea, Lubinski,& Benbow, 2001;Wai, Lubinski,& Benbow, 2009).1 EVIDENCE OFMALLEABILITY If spatial intelligence is theoretically andpractically important, the immediate question is whether it can be improved—can children be educated in a way that would maximize their potential in this domain? There is, happily, mounting evidence that spatial thinking can be developed a great deal. It turns out that even people who are considered spatially proficient are not nearly as proficient as they could be and that people who consider themselves spatially lacking can attain higher levels of performance. Most prior attempts to improve spatial skills via training have focused on transformation processes, such as the rotation and scaling of objects, rotation and scaling of arrays, and computation of the effects of viewer movement. A good deal of this research has involvedmental rotation, not only because it is an important process but also because there are detailed accounts of the relevant cognitive processes, some excellent tests of the ability, and intriguing (if dismaying) reports of gender differences (see Newcombe, 2002, for an overview). Results of individual training attempts have varied. Some researchers have claimed that practice leads people to make fundamental changes in how they process spatial stimuli, leading to transfer to novel stimuli and new tasks (Bethell-Fox&Shepard, 1988). Specifically, there are findings that improvement in spatial processing can generalize to novel stimuli within the same task (Leone, Taine, & Droulez, 1993), to other tasks of the samegeneral type (e.g.,mental rotation;DeLisi&Cammarano, 1996), and to tasks that share underlying cognitive processes with the practiced task (Wallace&Hofelich, 1992). However, others have reported that improvements in one spatial task do not transfer to other spatial tasks (Heil, Rösler, Link, & Bajric, 1998; Stericker&LeVesconte, 1982). In fact, practice has often been studied in paradigms using the same stimuli multiple times (Kail, 1986), thus leaving open the possibility that any gains are confined to a very narrow range of items. A panel convened by theNational Academy of Sciences concluded that transfer of spatial improvements has not been convincingly demonstrated and called for research aimed at determining how to improve spatial performance in a generalizable way (National Research Council, 2006). When viewed in the aggregate, however, the body of literature on spatial training is actually quite encouraging. Baenninger and Newcombe (1989) conducted a meta-analysis of studies of spatial training done up through the 1980s. They found very clear improvements in spatial ability that were, as one would expect, more striking as training was longer and more thorough. Research subsequent to the meta-analysis has supported these conclusions. For example, it has been shown that timeperiodswith greater amounts of school input (winter months) are associated with greater cognitive growth in the area of spatial operations in elementary school children than time periods with less school input (summer months) (Huttenlocher, Levine,&Vevea, 1998). Thus, it is likely that various educational techniques are benefiting children in the development of their spatial abilities. Additionally encouraging is the finding of a recent meta-analysis (Uttal, Hand, Meadow, & Newcombe, 2010) which includes studies completed since the Baenninger and Newcombe review. Again it was shown that there are substantial improvements in spatial skill from a wide variety of interventions, including academic coursework, task-specific practice, and playing computer games. To illustrate this literature, let us consider two studies (Terlecki, Newcombe, & Little, 2008; Wright, Thompson, Ganis, Newcombe, & Kosslyn, 2008) that gave undergraduates extended practice or training on mental rotation, for a period more prolonged than many other studies. The studies found that the training effects observed after practice lasted for the following several months and generalized to other spatial tasks (something that has rarely been observed). These effects were alsomassive—far larger in fact than the size of the typical sex difference. Terlecki et al. (2008) investigated longterm effects ofmental rotation training and addressedwhether these training effects are durable, transferable, larger for those who trained with videogames as opposed to simple practice, different for men and women, or different for individuals of higherand lower initial ability.Undergraduatesparticipated in semester-longweekly practicewith theMentalRotationsTest Volume 4—Number 3 103 Early Education for Spatial Intelligence (MRT) or, additionally, played the video-game Tetris. Structural equationmodeling showed large improvements inmental rotation with both test practice and video-game training; these gainsweremaintained several months later. Video-game training led to greater initial growth than practice alone, but final performance did not reliably differ. However, video-game training transferred to two other spatial tasks at levels exceeding the effects of practice; this transfer advantage was still evident after several months. MRT scores of men and women did not converge, but men showed faster initial growth and women showedmore improvement after the first fewweeks— especially women with lower levels of spatial experience. In a second training study, Wright et al. (2008) investigated whether intensive long-term practice leads to change that transcends stimuli and task parameters. College students were tested on three cognitive tasks: a computerized version of the Shepard-Metzler (Shepard &Metzler, 1971) MRT, a mental paper-folding task (MPFT), and a verbal analogies task. Each individual then participated in daily practice sessions with either the MRT or the MPFT over 21 days. Postpractice comparisons revealed transfer of practice gains tonovel stimuli for the practiced task as well as transfer to the other, nonpracticed, spatial task. The transfer was symmetric: as great from MRT to MPFT and vice versa. These findings indicate that practice improves performance on spatial tasks, beyond simply training for shortcuts that allow successful manipulation of specific stimuli. Improvement in the nonpracticed spatial task was greater than in the verbal analogies test, and thus improvement was not merely because of greater ease with computerized testing. Overall, it is clear that spatial training works, in a way that generalizes to new stimuli and novel tasks, and is durable over time. For participants of low ability, it appears that there is an initial hump to get over, but if they persevere through a period of slow improvement, faster improvement eventually occurs. These conclusions are not the end of the story, however. There is more to find out in future research. For example, how wide is transfer? Is improved spatial skill causally related to better STEM performance, and if so which specific skills translate to better performance in which subject areas? What are the neural correlates of better scores, and would such data give us new ideas about more targeted training methods? Of course, these studies involved adults. What about early spatial development? A focus on spatial skills should likely begin in the first 5 years of life, given evidence that early education generally pays the biggest dividends for later achievement (Heckman, 2006).What is the nature of early spatial processing, and how can caregivers and educators best interact with children to support their acquisition and development of these skills? Examiningwhat is known about the early development can provide a good basis for answering this question. Two particularly important and well-studied skills are the ability to imagine transformations of the orientation of objects (e.g., mental rotations) and the ability to imagine the consequenceof observer movements around arrays of objects (i.e., perspective taking). Mental rotation is the skill for which the strongest evidence exists currently for positing relations with STEM learning, and both mental rotation and perspective taking have been extensively studied from a variety of approaches, including research that has adopted developmental, cognitive, psychometric, and neuroscientific methods. There are interesting similarities and differences between mental rotation and perspective taking, illustrating the necessity of careful analysis of spatial skills in order to gain a deep understanding of spatial development. MENTAL TRANSFORMATIONS IN INFANTS AND PRESCHOOLERS There are two basic kinds of spatial transformations. On the one hand, one can mentally transform objects: for example, imagine objects changing their orientation when they rotate, changing scale by expansion or shrinkage, being cut in half or folded, and so on. On the other hand, one can imagine oneself, as the observer, taking new perspectives and moving with respect to objects and arrays of objects. Research on mental rotation (imagined object movements) and perspective taking (imagined observer movements) goes back to Piaget and Inhelder (1956, 1971). They proposed that initially only egocentric and static representations are available. According to them, it is not until 7–10 years of age, during the concrete operational stage, that children start to differentiate viewpoints and become able to represent movements of objects in space, manipulate mental images, and anticipate the outcome of events.However, subsequent studies on the early emergence and development of these abilities show that they can emerge earlier than Piaget and Inhelder claimed and also demonstrate considerable development across the preschool years. These descriptive facts are important forwell-informed intervention. Development of Mental Rotation Studies using looking-timeparadigmswith infants as young as 4 months have shown evidence for precursors of mental rotation (Hespos & Rochat, 1997; Rochat & Hespos, 1996). These paradigms have even detected early sex differences, with male infants showing more evidence of mental rotation than female infants (Moore & Johnson, 2008; Quinn & Liben, 2008). However, despite these interesting findings, data on very early mental rotation need to be interpreted with caution. Research paradigms used with infants differ from those used with older children and adults in several ways and do not necessarily measure the same ability. For instance, infants in many of the looking-time studies had the opportunity to watch a substantial proportion of the movement in the familiarization phase, whereas mental rotation paradigms used with older children 104 Volume 4—Number 3 Nora S. Newcombe and Andrea Frick and adults (Cooper&Shepard, 1973; Shepard&Metzler, 1971) typically present static stimuli. Thus, the infants did not have tomentally initiate the transformation, butmerely to continue and extrapolate an ongoing or recently presented movement. Mental continuation of movement may be easier than starting a mental transformation from a static state. In fact, Frick andWang (2010) showed that, although infants of 16 months looked longer at improbable outcomes of object rotations, even when they had to initiate the mental rotation, this pattern did not appear in 14-month-old infants, quite a lot older than the infantswhohave been argued to be showingmental rotation in the studies mentioned above. However, Frick and Wang also found that 14-month-olds who had less than 2 min of handson training with a turntable looked longer at the improbable outcome. This suggests that active motor experience increases infants’ ability to predict the outcome of transformations. The Frick and Wang (2010) experiments might be taken to indicate a substantial ability to mentally rotate objects in 14-month-olds, even though some prior experience might be necessary to activate this ability. However, there are other reasons to think that mental rotation is far from fully developed even in later infancy. Örnkloo and von Hofsten (2007) found that it was not until 22 months that infants could mentally rotate objects in order to successfully fit them through holes, and a recent study has also shown that there is considerable developmental progress in this ability from 15 to 30 months (Shutts, Örnkloo, von Hofsten, Keen, & Spelke, 2009). Research on mental rotation abilities has also revealed important individual differences in 3.5to 5.5-year-olds (Frick &Newcombe, 2009). Some children in this age range perform above chance on a mental rotation task using a touch screen paradigm. On the other hand, some children perform chance at level and show flat response time curves, suggesting that they donotmentally rotate the stimuli. These results challenge Marmor’s (1975, 1977) results, which she interpreted to suggest that, at the age of 4–5 years, children are able to perform mental rotations. Even though widely accepted at the time, there has always been some controversy about this conclusion (reviewed by Newcombe, 2002). Other studies have failed to replicate Marmor’s results (Dean&Harvey, 1979), and, in line with our results, analyses of individual children’s response time patterns (Estes, 1998) suggested that only a small proportion of 4-year-olds appeared to apply a mental rotation strategy. Marmor (1977) also found that training children to use a mental rotation strategy did not have a significant effect, from which she concluded that 4and 5-year-olds can spontaneously use and evoke mental rotations. However, a later replication study showed that, with training, twice as many 5-year-olds produced linear reaction time patterns that are indicative of a mental rotation strategy (Platt & Cohen, 1981). Furthermore, mental rotation has been shown to continuously strengthen through early childhood (Estes, 1998; Levine, Huttenlocher, Taylor, & Langrock, 1999; Okamoto-Barth & Call, 2008). Several recent studies with children in kindergarten through elementary school suggest that motor activity and what has been called ‘‘embodied thinking’’ may play an influential role especially in young children’s mental transformation abilities. These studies showed similar developmental trends in the degree to which mental rotation was impaired by concurrent incompatible manual rotations (Frick, Daum, Walser, &Mast, 2009) and hand postures (Funk, Brugger, & Wilkening, 2005), or in how mental spatial transformations were facilitated by concurrent compatible hand movements (Frick, Daum,Wilson, &Wilkening, 2009). Development of Perspective Taking There are several kinds of perspective taking, known to be graded in difficulty (for overviews of this literature, see Newcombe, 1988; Newcombe & Huttenlocher, 2000). The easiest kind of perspective taking involves predicting what will be seen after an actual physical movement. This prediction requires spatial memory and transformation when the target object or array is hidden, but thephysicalmovement also transforms relations in regards to the frame of reference, making the task easier. The task is more difficult when the observer does not physicallymove butmust imagine hypothetical movement (Huttenlocher & Presson, 1973). In infant studies, there isusually actualmovementaroundan array,whichmakesperspective takingeasier.Roughlyatanage when infants are becoming more mobile and are able to move around on their own, they begin to develop some perspectivetaking ability when they move. For instance, a study with 8-month-olds (Bai & Bertenthal, 1992) showed that infants’ locomotor status predicted their ability to keep track of the location of an object when they changed their position. In this study, a toywas hidden in one of two identical wells on a table and the infants searched for the toy after being moved to the opposite side of the table. Creeping infants (infants who were able tomove onhands andkneeswithout their bellies touching the floor) predominantly searched in the correctwell, whereas crawling (with bellies touching the floor) and precrawling infants showed below-chance searches. Furthermore, two experimental studies indicate that infants arebetter atkeeping track of a hidden object if they themselves actively move to a new position. In one study (Acredolo, Adams, & Goodwyn, 1984), infantswere trainedat 12 and 18 months tofindanobject hidden in one of two identical wells in a Plexiglas box. When allowed to search for the toy from the opposite side of the box, correct searches predominated at 18 months. A similar study (Benson & Uzgiris, 1985) showed that even 10to 11-monthold infants predominantly searched in the correct well, if they actively crawled to the newvantage point, as opposed to being passively carried there by a parent. Newcombe, Huttenlocher, Drummey, andWiley (1998) askedchildrenages 16–36 months to search for objects hidden in a rectangular sandbox, after Volume 4—Number 3 105 Early Education for Spatial Intelligence they had walked to the opposite side of the box. In this study, children moved in a curtained environment and had to search for the object in a homogenous space, as opposed to the studies above that used two distinct hiding places. Hence children needed to rely primarily on dead reckoning. Nevertheless, children across this age range performed above chance. Whereas in the studies described above the observers actually moved to a new location, other research has directly investigated imagined observer transformations in young children.Rieser,Garing, andYoung(1994)testedwhetherchildren were able to imagine a distant spatial layout and then imagine a change in perspective. Children sat at home and were asked to imagine how their classroom would look like, first from their own seat, and then from their teacher’s seat, and to point to different locations in their classroom. Children at 3.5 years and older were accurate and rapid in their pointing, but only if they were instructed to imagine walking from their seat to the teacher’s seat while actually walking a similar path and turning consistently with the to-be-imagined heading. Even though the above studies reveal surprising abilities at very young ages, the tasks are quite different from the classic Piagetian perspective-taking tasks (e.g., Three Mountains Task: Piaget & Inhelder, 1948/1956), in which children had to choose which of several pictures would show what they would see if theymoved around an array. In Rieser et al.’s task, there isminimal competing sensory inputbecauseperspectives were imagined in a dark room. In Piaget and Inhelder’s picture selection task, there is a competition between the perceptually present surround and the one that must be imagined. There are other ways than turning out the lights to reduce this competition. Newcombe and Huttenlocher (1992) asked preschoolers to imagine how a layout located immediately in front of them would look from different viewpoints, using a verbal question that highlighted a particular spatial relation (What object would be closest to you?). Their results showed that 3to 5-year-olds were able to indicate the location of objects relative to another viewpoint, although the 3-yearolds often responded egocentrically, that is, relative to their own current perspective. However, children of this age were completely unable to cope with Piaget and Inhelder’s original task: selecting which of four pictures would show the array from the imagined perspective. In a study with older children that provided the basis for the later study of preschoolers, Huttenlocher and Presson (1973) found that even thirdand fifthgraders still made numerous egocentric errors in classic perspective-taking tasks using arrays of pictures, although they performed much better in response to verbal questions that focused on particular spatial relations than with the picture-selection task. Thus, it appears that the real challenge in perspective-taking tasks during the school years is not the mental transformation that underlies the ability to imagine someone else’s perspective per se, but ignoring one’s own perspective and perceptual surroundings. Mental Rotation Versus Perspective Taking Huttenlocher and Presson (1973, 1979) showed that performance in observer rotation (or perspective-taking) tasks differed considerably from performance in array rotation (or mental rotation) tasks and that the degree of difficulty in the two tasks was not influenced by the same factors. These results have been corroborated by further behavioral studies that showed performance differences between observer rotation and array or object rotation tasks in adults. For instance, different response time functions have been found in object rotation tasks and observer rotation tasks. Object rotations typically show a linear increase in response time as a function of angle of rotation; that is, larger rotations typically take longer (Cooper & Shepard, 1973; Shepard & Metzler, 1971). On the other hand, when participants are instead asked to imagine themselves rotating, this linear increase in reaction times has not always been found (Jola & Mast, 2005; Wraga, Creem, & Proffitt, 2000; Zacks, Mires, Tversky, & Hazeltine, 2000). In a factor analytic study, Hegarty and Waller (2004) comparedseveralmeasuresofmental rotationandperspectivetaking abilities and concluded that measures of perspective taking and mental rotation are dissociable, while correlated, in adults. They argued that these two types of spatial transformations rely on different cognitive operations, although they may also share some common processes, such as generating and maintaining mental representations (Kosslyn, 1994). This research raises the question of whether imagined observer rotations and object rotations recruit the same brain areas. Evidence from neurophysiological studies in adults suggests that tasks that require object-based spatial transformations and those that require viewpoint changes depend on different neural processes (Creem et al., 2001; Kosslyn, Digirolamo, Thompson, & Alpert, 1998; Zacks, Rypma, Gabrieli, Tversky, & Glover, 1999). For instance, lesions to right posterior cortex were associated with selective impairments at object rotation tasks, whereas lesions to left posterior cortex were associated with selective impairments in the ability to navigate and to imagine oneself turning, as in following a route on a map (for a review see Zacks et al., 1999).

برای دانلود رایگان متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Aging, Pensions and Long-term Care: What, Why, Who, How?; Comment on “Financing Long-term Care: Lessons From Japan”

Japan has been aging faster than other industrialized nations, and its experience offers useful lessons to others. Japan has been willing to expand its welfare state with a long-term care (LTC) insurance to finance home care and nursing home care for frail elderly. As Ikegami shows, it created new facilities and expanded specialized staffing for home care, developed a c...

متن کامل

Gender: An Emotional Intelligence Perspective

Dear Editor:    The rising importance of emotional intelligence gives a clear idea of the significant differences between men and women, since men and women are emotionally intelligent in different ways. Understanding and appreciating such differences leads to the improvement of the existing relationships and to the establishment of new relationships in a satisfying manner.       One of the pri...

متن کامل

Comparison of Effectiveness of Presenting Images in Visual Education of Students on Brain Function

Background & Aim: Instead of being based on reality, theoretical approaches related to the occurrence of learning are more based on the observations by researchers. One of the approaches is the study of the effect of changes in virtual education based on the physiological approach. This study aimed to compare the effectiveness of presentation of images in visual education on the brain function ...

متن کامل

Analytical Computing for Spatial Design: An Artificial Intelligence Perspective

Next-generation people-centred design systems, frameworks, assistive tools, educational aids, and design policies necessitate foundational abstraction and computational building blocks where the modalities of human perception, action, environmental experience, and design conception and semantics are central. Our research in this context addresses the following questions: – Contemporary CAAD too...

متن کامل

Why Good Quality Care Needs Philosophy More Than Compassion; Comment on “Why and How Is Compassion Necessary to Provide Good Quality Healthcare?”

Although Marianna Fotaki’s Editorial is helpful and challenging by looking at both the professional and institutional requirements for reinstalling compassion in order to aim for good quality healthcare, the causes that hinder this development remain unexamined. In this commentary, 3 causes are discussed; the boundary between the moral and the political; Neoliberalism; and the underdevelopment ...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2010